Over 400,000 people visited Farnam Street last month to learn how to make better decisions, create new ideas, and avoid stupid errors. With more than 100,000 subscribers to our popular weekly digest, we've become an online intellectual hub. To learn more about we what do, start here.

Remembering the Future

Insights about the role of memory and attention in decision making may help us make wiser choices — putting future considerations ahead of immediate gratification.

 

A lengthy excerpt:

Not only do people have a tendency to think about the present at the expense of the future, but when asked to put off consumption, they demand disproportionately large rewards for doing so. “If a customer expects to receive an iPad immediately and then Apple announces there’s been a delay and offers a coupon for other products as a compensation for the wait, most people would expect a lot for that,” Weber says. “But if a customer expects to receive an iPad in three weeks and Apple offers the chance to get it sooner for a premium, most people would not be willing to pay that much more.”
 

“Economic theory says that people should be willing to pay as much to speed up their consumption as they would demand to slow it down,” says Professor Eric Johnson, whose research focuses on consumer and managerial decision making. But these amounts are never the same, and in fact, the difference is startling: the price people demand to slow down consumption is on average two to three times higher than the price people will pay to speed it up. The phenomenon — asymmetric discounting — has long been noted but has not been well-explained. “Understanding why asymmetric discounting happens might help us understand how to make immediate gratification look smaller and distant rewards look bigger,” Johnson says.

 

“Other work on decision making shows that people construct what they are willing to pay,” he explains, “meaning that our preferences are often more a matter of the way a choice is presented than deeply held beliefs or values.” For example, people share a well-documented tendency to favor the first option among many choices. In earlier research, Johnson and others proposed query theory, the idea that order matters in preferences and decision making because it affects how we query our memory for arguments for different choices; our brains give first choices more attention than subsequent choices, and we generate more arguments for them.

 

Like first options, the present is always directly in front of us, demanding — and receiving — attention, while the future is difficult to recall because it is so much further away. “So the question is, how do we focus attention on future rewards and move them to the forefront?” Johnson says.

 

Johnson and Weber, working with Kerry Milch and Hannah Chang of Columbia University, Jeff Brodscholl of Harris Interactiveand Daniel Goldstein of London Business School, hypothesized that query theory could reveal whether those cognitive processes associated with memory and attention were at work in asymmetric discounting and the tendency to favor the present over the future. The researchers proposed that, because the order in which people receive their options matters, they should be able to shift the balance of support from the present to the future by prompting people to generate more evidence in favor of delaying rewards and less evidence in favor of immediate rewards.

 

The researchers first conducted a standard asymmetric discounting study with subjects on the Web with real money at stake. Half of the participants were offered the chance to receive a $75 gift certificate three months from that day, while half were presented with the chance to receive a $50 gift certificate immediately. Consistent with other measures of discount rates and asymmetric discounting, the subjects offered the $75 (later) option were willing, on average, to give up about $15 to receive the gift certificate right away, while those offered the $50 (now) option demanded an increase of $30 to wait three months.

 

Subjects had also been asked to report (by typing) the thoughts that were running through their minds as they were making their decisions. Later, after making their choices, subjects categorized their thoughts as supporting immediate consumption or supporting delayed consumption (or none or both). By counting the number of thoughts in each category, the researchers determined whether the balance of support in each instance favored immediate or delayed consumption.

 

As the researchers had hypothesized, subjects who had first been given the chance to receive the $75 gift certificate in the future were able to list more thoughts supporting delayed consumption. Those first offered the chance to receive a $50 gift certificate in the present reported many more thoughts that favored immediate consumption.

 

This lends credence to the theory that people muster the most evidence to support the first choice they are offered, drowning out other options. “We think something like this happens: someone offered $75 to spend three months in the future might first think about how their spouse’s birthday is right around that time, or maybe a holiday, and they think about what kind of nice gift they might buy,” Weber explains. “Whereas someone offered $50 to spend today might first think about that CD box set they’ve been eyeing. Subsequent ideas have little chance of overriding the first one.”

Continue Reading

Date:
Filed Under: